The Effects of Art Education on the Development of Cognitive Skills Nita Sturiale Introduction As an artist, a student of science, and a teacher, I have long been interested in the intersections between thinking artistically and thinking scientifically as well as the ramifications for this intersection in education. I use the discoveries, data and vocabularies of science, particularly neuroscience, as the medium for my artwork. Additionally, I have practiced in art education and have observed remarkable learning in students provided with the opportunity to explore artistic discovery alongside more traditional skill acquisition. It is my deeply held belief that integrating these two different modes of learning is beneficial for all. I am delighted with this opportunity to investigate the neurological basis for this hypothesis. There have been many efforts in recent years to save or reinstate art education in the public school system (Gardner, 1990, 1982,1973, Welch, 1995). These efforts claim that art education improves student performance. What are the causal connections between artistic experience and the development of other cognitive abilities? The purpose of this paper is to suggest that the study of art exercises, strengthens, and develops the neurophysiological areas and associations of the brain that are involved with other cognitive abilities. Evidence is presented that supports more inclusion of arts training in educational settings. Due to the brevity of this presentation, there is a focus on the use of music in education, though it is the belief of this author that all other areas of artistic experience could make similar claims. This paper is divided into five sections. The first section, What is Music?, identifies the components of music in order to more accurately compare the complex functions associated with musical experience to other cognitive functions. Secondly, Is Music Experience Localized In the Right Hemisphere of the Brain?, this sections reviews the literature surrounding the laterality of the brain in an effort to clarify some of the oversimplifications that have been widely accepted on this issue. Next, Are there Developmental Patterns of Hemispheric Activity?, this section introduces theories and experimental evidence that suggest the two hemispheres develop both separately and together in a cycling pattern that repeats itself over time. In the section entitled, Does Musical Experience Influence Cognitive Skills?, experimental evidence is presented that demonstrates the direct influence of musical training on the improvement of other skills. And finally, the section, How Can Recent Brain Research Influence Education? proposes several possible results of exposure to arts education during development. What is Music? Music is particularly relevant to this discussion because it is a complex symbolic system, much like language and mathematics. The areas of the brain used in music perception, appreciation and expression (many, but not all, thought to be localized in the right hemisphere) are also needed for the visual-spatial skills necessary for mathematical processing. There is historical as well as experimental evidence for this math/music connection. The human experience and expression of music begins for almost every one prenatally (Ostwald & Morrison, 1988). In development, musical interests, abilities and perception have very early origins in the womb. The sounds and rhythms of the mother surround the infant long before it is exposed to any visual stimuli. A mother or caretaker's voice becomes very important in developing and maintaining the mother/infant bond. Information contained in the musical properties of a mother's voice perhaps develop both the musical and linguistic areas of the brain at this early stage. Many have attempted to define the components of music. For the purposes of this paper, I will focus on three author's opinions. In Henson's essay in the collection, Music and the Brain (Critchley & Henson, 1977), he says that the expressive or receptive experience of music depends upon one's ability to perceive the following: the specific acoustic properties of a note; tones and tonal relationships (pitch, duration, timbre, and intensity); the sounding of notes consecutively, simultaneously and in terms of rhythm; melody; harmony; and the horizontal relationship of tones (counterpoint). Memory and emotional response is also necessary for a full experience of music (Henson, 1977). More recently, McAdams (1996) has attempted to further deconstruct the experience of music. From his research into the cognitive psychology of music, he focuses on the complexity of structural and symbolic processing necessary for musical experience and believes it "rivals that of linguistic processing" (McAdams, pg. 275). He explains that humans possess elaborate and longterm representations of musical/sound abstractions dependent upon cultural, genetic and linguistic influence. These abstractions begin to form from the first experience of processing auditory stimulation. The listener interprets the value and function of these different events according to a rich and complex system of coding and elaboration. The resulting codes are context dependent and in this way similar to mathematical and linguistic processing. Wertheim's (1977) description focuses on the importance of rhythm. He believes that rhythm is the framework of music. It provides the melody and the context dependent information needed to perceive the information properly. The same score played with a different rhythm can have very different emotional content. The differences in tonal combinations and the timing of rhythms can be represented by mathematical relationships. He states that any single sound is an event in time and in this way music can be described accordingly. To perceive music is to perceive time. Simultaneity, rhythm, movement, pitch, duration, and the relationships between all these factors can symbolize abstract emotions and ideas. He writes, "Music cannot exist without time. the pitch of sounds, their different duration, the development of melody, all rely on the element of time which is measured and divided into patterns by what we call rhythm" (Wertheim, 1977). The frequencies, sound waves, and rhythms of music can be represented mathematically. In order to appreciate the complexity of a particular musical score, a listener must possess a cognitive system for representing the relationships between these frequencies and sounds. In these ways, music and math are closely related. Is Music Experience Localized In the Right Hemisphere in the Brain? Because of this close relationship of music and math, it is interesting that the processing of these two activities is attributed to distant locations in the brain. Many functions involved in mathematical, analytic and sequential thinking have been localized in the Left Hemisphere (LH), whereas the functions associated with artistic, musical and holistic thinking have been demonstrated by the Right Hemisphere (RH). In order to study the effects of music on the brain, I will briefly elaborate on the RH/LH discussion. The functional separation of the RH and LH has sent us down less than reliable paths and many have attempted to set the record straighter (Churchland, 1890; Springer & Deutsch, 1985; Joseph, R., 1988). While others, like the author of Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain, have capitalized on the idea by designing brain booster systems for getting more out of an individual half. A selection of views are presented to clarify this issue. It is known that there are definite differences between the hemispheres in the general right-handed population (Springer & Deutsch, 1985). It is safe to say that this is indisputable. But the nature of these differences is still in question (Churchland, 1986). In the Journal of Clinical Psychology, Joseph provides a summative review of the literature on laterality(1988). He provides a long list of functions with which the RH has been shown to dominate: the perception and identification of environmental and nonverbal sounds; somesthesis; steriogenesis; the maintenance of the body image; the comprehension and expression of prosodic, melodic, and emotional features of speech; the analysis of geometric and visual-space; the production of certain forms of visual images; dreams during REM sleep; the perception and expression of visual, facial and verbal affect; the ability to determine a person's mood, attitude and intentions via the analysis of gesture, facial expression, vocal-melodic and intonational qualities; social-emotional functioning; and finally, the perception of most aspects of music (Joseph, 1988). Joseph states, "Although there is evidence of considerable functional overlap as well as inter-hemispheric cooperation on a number of tasks, it certainly appears that the mental system maintained by the right hemisphere is highly developed, social-emotional, bilateral, and in many ways dominant over the temporal-sequential, language-dependent half of the cerebrum." (Joseph, pg. 659). This is quite a different picture then the idea that the RH is the subordinate to the dominant LH. Another view is that of Cornock (1984). He warns against making too many conclusions about functional hemispheric laterality based primarily on split brain research. He explains that many of these subjects have many other problems (epilepsy) that may contribute to a relocation of some functions. He also discusses the difficulty in localizing right hemisphere functions as they seem to be more diffuse and integrative. What he will attribute to the RH are the functions of facilitating the immediate recognition of relationships and significant patterns, visuospatial skills, the figurative use and interpretation of language (humor), attaching emotional content to phenomena, performing parallel rather than sequential processing, enabling the appreciation of events, and musical awareness (Cornock, 1984). More specifically concerned with the laterality differences between musicians and non-musicians Hassler(1990) studied five groups of subjects for lateral dominance, musical talent, spatial processing, handedness, verbal processing, psychological androgyny, and physiological androgyny (Hassler, 1990). One group consisted of musical composers, another group was made up of instrumentalists, the third group were all non-musicians, a fourth group of painters and finally the last group consisted of non musicians with low educational status. Each group was an even mix of males and females. The results demonstrated that females in the artistic groups were more strongly lateralized than both male and female non-musicians. The males in the artistic groups were less lateralized than non musicians. Their data support the assumption that LH and RH functions contributing to processes associated with verbal processing are more effectively integrated in musicians than in non-musicians. These studies confirm earlier research by Hassler that musicians have enhanced spatial abilities compared to non musicians regardless of gender. Hassler goes on to claim that anomalous dominance is assumed to favor special talents (music, math and spatial skills ) but also related to developmental learning disorders. Other, more popular (Shreeve, 1996), examples of the lateralization of musical skills include that of the Russian composer Vissarion Shebelin, who suffered two left hemisphere strokes. Afterwards, he was unable to speak or understand the meaning of words, yet continued to compose and teach music. Another composer, Maurice Ravel, began to make spelling mistakes and eventually lost his ability to read and could no longer sign his name. Yet, unlike Shebelin he could no longer compose, though he persistently said that he had a new opera "in his head". He could still play scales and listen and enjoy musical performances. These different situations suggest the close proximity of areas in the brain that are related to music composition and linguistic abilities, yet they are still separate. An early example of using EEG technology to explore the laterality question in relation to musical experience and behavior is the work done by Davidson & Schwartz (1977). They measured the EEG activity of subjects while they remembered and reproduced music with and without lyrics. All the subjects were right handed. 9 males and 5 females, a mix of both musically trained and untrained subjects, were asked to list 3 familiar songs before the test. They were then asked to first, whistle a melody, then talk the lyrics to a song and finally sing a song. Each task was recorded for one minute with eyes closed. This scenario was repeated twice. Once for recordings done in two LH and RH parietal locations and then again for two occipital locations. The subjects who were not musically trained showed more activity in the RH while whistling vs. talking the lyrics. The musically trained subjects showed no difference. Also, there were no differences between groups during talking or singing. The authors state that their data are consistent with recent evidence suggesting that musical training is associated with the adoption of an analytic and sequential processing mode toward melodic information (Davidson & Schwartz, 1977). Zatorre (Shreeve, 1996) has visualized the differences between the hemispheres while they were in action using PET scanning techniques. While Subjects listened to a tune , these scans show activity in the right superior temporal gyrus. When asked to pay special attention to the particular pitches within the tunes and make comparisons, the scans show activity in both the RH and the LH. In addition to EEG and PET scanning technologies, Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, & Steinmetz (1995) show magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) evidence for increased lateralization in the left planum temporale in musicians with perfect pitch (Shreeve, 1996). Finally, Damasio & Damasio (1977), present evidence for a dynamic, developing cerebral dominance for certain features of musical faculty. They suggest that there is a RH dominance for musical execution (regardless of training) and a variable dominance for musical perception, starting in the RH in the musically naive and developing into a LH dominance in the musically sophisticated. In summary, many particular functions have been found to be localized in different hemispheres. Musical experience is generally more localized in the RH in naive listeners but in both hemispheres in trained musicians. The developing perception of music seems to involve both hemispheres and increased skill level coincides with an increase in the integration between the two hemispheres. This relocation of cognitive processing associated with music coincides with recent thoughts about developmental stages in skill acquisition. Are there Developmental Patterns of Hemispheric Activity? An exciting recent discovery is that of the cyclic patterns of electrical activity throughout the brain during development. It has been shown that a cycle of activity beginning with a relative increase of electrical activity in one hemisphere, then this increase moves to the other hemisphere, ending with an increase of electrical activity bilaterally and in parallel. This cycle then repeats itself. It is hypothesized that this pattern begins in infanthood and continues into adulthood. In the collection, Human Behavior and the Developing Brain (Fischer & Dawson, 1994), a wealth of research is presented that supports a new theoretical framework upon which new data confirming the cyclic activity throughout development can be placed. Fischer & Rose (1994) are concerned that the current framework of Piagetian stage theory does not allow for the complex amount of variation that the current data exhibit. They are attempting to connect the patterns of growth in both neuro-development and behavior. There research suggests that in cognition there are at least 13 developmental levels. Each level is dependent on acquiring a new type of control system to coordinate new skills. This control system is correlated with brain growth spurts which provide the new neural networks necessary for a new control system. Each new control system, or capacity, is possible due to an increased coordination between previous skills creating a more complex skill. In their research, brain growth spurts are identified by EEG measurements and head growth findings. There are correlation's between this data and the ages of cognitive and emotional developments. These new control systems and increased coordination between skills of previous control systems are referred to as levels and tiers. A level of skill development is created by a growth cycle in network connectivity. A tier is indicated by a growth cycle in location and extent of EEG activity. At each level there is a cycle of activity. First connectivity grows in the one hemisphere then in the other and finally in both in parallel. This cycles repeats for every level of skill development (Fischer & Rose, 1994). This theoretical framework is supported by Thatcher's (1994) work analyzing cortical EEG coherence cycles over time. Thatcher measured 436 children and young adults from the age of 6 months to 16 years. He postulates that there are 3 cycles of growth spurts of cortical connections during this time period. These are defined by a sequential lengthening of intra-cortical connections in the LH accompanied by a sequential contraction of intra-cortical connections in the RH. This research shows that there is a direction to development and that the two hemispheres develop at different times. Thatcher suggests that this difference is due to the different functions of the hemispheres. The LH sequence is from short distance connections to long distance integrations which may mature into the analytical and sequential processing observed in the adult LH. Whereas the RH sequence is from long distance to short, mirroring the spatial, holistic, and integrative functions of the adult RH. Following are three theories that attempt to answer the question of which hemisphere these cycles begin in. Goldberg & Costa (1981) present relevant neuroanatomical evidence for a right to left shift of the relative hemispheric control over particular cognitive skills over the course of development. These authors suggest that the LH achieves the ability to use many and varied descriptive systems which have already been fully formed in an individuals "cognitive repertoire". The RH is crucial for dealing with novelty or any materials or tasks that existing descriptive systems do not account for. The RH creates new descriptive systems to deal with these. They also suggest that the LH is primarily involved in developing modality specific cortical areas and integrating within them, unimodal and motor processing and the storage of compact codes. The RH develops associative areas, intermodal integration and the processing of novel stimuli. The authors(Goldberg & Costa, 1981) show that the RH functions are primarily involved in the processing of unfamiliar and novel stimuli and the LH functions are involved when stimuli have become familiar and are processed more automatically. This work suggests that the movement of developmental activity in the cortex moves from the RH to the LH. Licht, Bakker, Kok, & Bouma (1988) extended these ideas and attempted to find electrical activity correlates for this shift described above. Their goals were to measure age-related changes in ERP components that are elicited by words in young children over a four year period beginning with kindergarten. A relationship was found between reading performance and ERP amplitudes over the right parietal hemisphere in young children and over the left temporal hemisphere in older children. A particular ERP component (slow wave) was observed and found to be associated with an increase in processing difficulty. This component was more pronounced in young readers in the LH and in the RH in older readers. These ERP results again suggest that the RH is more involved at an earlier stage in development followed by the LH (Licht, Bakker, Kok, & Bouma, 1988) . A third study begins to question the above results, but reveals the importance of the RH in more complex cognitive levels. Roberts & Kraft (1989) tested 55 right handed boys who were above average readers from two age groups (grades 1-2 and 5-6). They read short passages and were asked questions based on the reading. EEG measures were taken during the silent reading. This research found a relationship between hemispheric activation and age difference. LH activation was associated with younger subjects while bilateral processing was demonstrated in older readers. These authors (Roberts & Kraft,1989) propose that this represents the increased comprehension in older readers. Older children rely on a more "dynamic flow of complex strategies, which involves greater inter-hemispheric integration" (Roberts & Kraft, pg. 326). The expected shift toward good reading comprehension being increasingly associated with LH activation was NOT found. This was only true with the beginning readers. Older children that showed bilateral activation patterns got significantly higher inferential reading comprehension scores then those with an increase in LH activation. These results suggest that there is more LH processing at early stages of development while the RH is relatively more involved in complex skills. In summary, there are developmental patterns in hemispheric activity throughout development. The RH and LH are both equally involved in the development of cognitive skills but each develop at different times. It has also been shown that the RH is involved in more complex cognitive skills previously thought to be predominantly handled by the LH. Does Musical Experience Influence Cognitive Skills? In Nancy Welch's School's Communities and the Arts: A Research Compendium, there are summaries of at least 50 research projects each attempting to prove that arts education is valuable and necessary for students to reach their potentials. The organization and volume of these summaries is impressive at first glance, but upon closer look one finds that the direct causal relationships between art education and brain development is lacking. Yet, clearly art education does influence a students success. One example of a persuasive study in this compendium is that done Carolyn Hudspeth (1986). Two 4th grade language arts classes of low achievers were tested. Each class of 16 students was from a different school though closely matched in socio-economic and achievement levels. The California Achievement Test was used before and after the experiment to assess the influence of an arts education program. One class was taught a traditional language arts program while the other was taught with the SAMPLE method (Suggested Activities of Music and Poetry for Language Enrichment) designed by Hudsepth (1986). The results were positive: SAMPLE classes outperformed the traditional class by 5 years in "language mechanics" and 2.7 years on "total language" (Hudspeth, 1986) More recently, more controlled efforts at finding a causal link between music and education are being published that better bridge the gaps between the fields of educational research, cognitive psychology and brain development. In Finland, Kalliopuska & Ruokonen (1993) tested the effects of music exercises in the holistic development of empathy and presociability. Empathy was used as the skill to observe because the authors assume it is an integration of affective, cognitive, kinesthetic and physiological components that can be differentiated upon testing. It is a good example of holistic thinking and behavior. For their work 2 groups of 6 year olds were tested on their ability to think empathetically in several varied social situations and problems. One group attended a special Saturday music program for 12 weeks that met for one hour where subjects were involved in singing, playing instruments, listening, music exercise, and discussion about the emotions associated with their musical experiences. The control group had no such training. Before and after the training period subjects were tested with several empathy tests, including evaluations by their parents and teachers. The subjects were tested after 3 months of training and again after 9 months. There was substantial improvement in empathy test scores though these improvements subsided after 9 months and receded to almost the same as control group (Kalliopuska & Ruokonen, 1993). In 1996, Gardiner, Fox, Knowles & Jeffry conducted an similar experiment testing mathematics and reading skills. 96 students in 8 different classrooms participated. Four classrooms were dubbed 'test arts' rooms and these were taught the Kodaly method of music and visual arts curriculum which emphasized sequenced skill development (Barkoczi, 1987). The remaining classrooms participated in the standard art curriculum. Other curricula was the identical for all classrooms. After 7 months all students took standardized achievement tests. Students in the test arts classes had been behind controls in the previous year but after the 7 months were at least equal and often ahead in reading skills and ahead on learning mathematics. From the report, "Learning arts skills forces mental 'stretching' useful to other areas of learning: the maths learning advantage in our data could, for example, reflect the development of mental skills such as ordering, and other elements of thinking on which mathematical learning at this age also depends" (Gardiner et al., pg. 284). They make a direct relationship between the musical experience and the developmental skills needed to solve mathematical problems. The so-called "Mozart Effect" (Rideout, 1997) that has been described in various popular media circles (Shreeve, 1996) is the result of research initiated by Frances Rauscher and others that attempt to probe deeper into the general positive results described above. Rauscher recognizes the lack of causal evidence for the relationship between music cognition and other higher brain functions and for years she has been working on just that. This Mozart Effect was coined after Rauscher presented evidence that showed subjects improved on their ability to solve spatial reasoning problems after listening to a Mozart sonata (Rauscher, Shaw, & Ky, 1993). This causal relationship was demonstrated by testing 36 college students after they listened 10 minutes each of; Mozart's sonata for two pianos in D major; a relaxation tape; silence. After each listening time they were given standard IQ spatial reasoning tasks. Performance improved immediately following the Mozart sonata but not after the other two listening conditions. Arousal was discounted as the cause as pulse was also measured and no change was found. The effect lasted for 10 or 15 minutes but subsided thereafter. In order to experiment with more lasting effect, Rauscher also did another similar study where musical training of preschoolers was shown to improve spatial processing in a more permanent manner over a period of months (Rauscher, Shaw, Levine, Ky, & Wright, 1994). Rideout & Laubach expanded Rauscher's research ideas in 1996 by using EEG technology to measure this Mozart Effect. This study had two purposes. The authors wanted to replicate the improved spatial performance following exposure to music in adults and to examine the EEG correlates of performance changes after listening to music. They were interested in the whether specific association exist between changes in EEG characteristics and changes in performance on the spatial task. For this study, four men and four woman with a mean age of 21.1, each having no more than two years of music study were tested after listening to music and after listening to a relaxation tape. Again, spatial reasoning was tested after listening times. The EEG was recorded while they engaged in the spatial reasoning tasks. Spatial performance was again much improved. Subjects with generally lower alpha peak frequencies and higher beta were more likely to improve performance. This increased separation between peak frequencies may imply easier frequency discrimination. This may, in turn, facilitate music's enhancement of firing patterns used in spatial reasoning. Also, improved performance was correlated with increased alpha power in the left temporal area. The results suggest that the music had its effect by facilitating specific changes in brain state and associated EEG power which mediated improved performance. They hypothesized that the similar cognitive enhancements that are shown in spatial reasoning in children and in adults may be due to different developmental mechanisms (Rideout & Laubach, 1996). Though the sample tested in the research was much smaller than Rauscher's, this experiment stands as a model for further study into finding a direct causal connection between brain and behavior relationships. In summary, there is correlational and causal evidence for the improvement of cognitive skills after musical training. How Can Evidence for the Success of Arts Training Influence Education? Harth(1993) writes that spatial and temporal integration occurs in order to qualitatively perceive stimuli. Understanding speech, mathematical and spatial problems, or appreciating music would not be possible without this process, "It is not the musical note or chord picked up by your ear at this instant that makes it joyful or sad, but the fact that it is embedded in the past and borne by the anticipation of the future" (Harth, pg. 94). This poetic description is important in that it speaks of the possibility that artistic experience exercises the ability to hold together memory, anticipation and subsequent behavior. This is a cognitive skill that integrates the complex and seemingly limitless number of different brain functions. Schlaug & Heine have produced MRIs that show a larger corpus callosum in musicians who had trained from an early age in comparison to non-musicians (Shreeve, 1996). This suggests that early musical training enhances communication between the two hemispheres. This idea has profound ramifications for education, learning and development. Art training perhaps increases the inter-hemispheric connectivity that in turn influences one's ability to imagine the future, plan ahead, and desire understanding (curiosity). These abilities in turn help a learner in all subjects. Stimulating the RH during development enables better integration between the hemispheres and more complex coordination of particular functions. Most importantly it increases the integration of one's more abstract mental capacities for memory, anticipation, volition and behavior (Elliot, 1986). Additionally, musical training may increase a student's chances for becoming gifted in a particular area. Recent findings point to the prevalence of increased RH activity in gifted individuals. Oboyle, Alexander & Benbow (1991) suggest that students who are particularly precocious in math show an unexpected increase of EEG activity in their RH when involved in solving difficult problems. In the report of their research, Oboyle, et al. equate math precocity with above average intelligence in general. Their work shows that both LH and RH are involved in analytical skills for those that are particularly gifted (Oboyle, et al.1991). If particularly creative artistic and gifted intellectual ability involve an increased integration of the specialized functions of both hemispheres then the lack of emphasis on arts training demonstrated by the current educational system is problematic. This system does not exercise those functions found to reside more often in the RH as much as it exercises the LH functions. The educational system is only watering half the garden. Currently, school activities are limited to performance, production, and results. Current educational systems want students to learn how to quickly manipulate linguistic and mathematical symbol systems. However, the well-rounded development of cognition is a much more complex endeavor. Due to a lack of public understanding, appreciation, or support for art, and the fact that teachers are often unprepared to teach it, art offerings in schools are few. These few offerings decrease by the middle school years and by high school many, if not most, students have no art experience (Welch, 1995). When properly taught, art training is less about the product and more about the ideas, the individual's questions and the creative process. There is much potential for a student when this kind of education is coupled with linguistic and mathematical training. Robert Ornstein has written about his concern for the lack of educational systems that honor the total "mental operating system" of the mind. His theory describes the mind as an entity capable of multiple simultaneous operations, each concerned with a different discipline (Vincent & Merrion, 1990). This view is also held with variations by other contemporary authors and educational theorists, (Gardner, 1990; Goleman, 1995). Ornstein is concerned that the valuing mind, distinct from the reasoning mind, is not well developed in our current verbal/linguistic/mathematics based educational system. The study of music (and other art forms) is a prime experience for developing the valuing mind. The experience of music allows for the practice of feeling many complex emotions at once. Practice is safer than the real thing and prepares an individual to better deal with real events when they arise. Music training improves the many capacities of the mental operating system- mathematical, emotional, moral and social. In summary it is proposed that musical training has longterm effect on abstract reasoning skills, the integrative capacities of the brain, and volition in learning, especially when exposed at the right developmental stage. Conclusions In general, this paper has presented references that show that the RH is more involved in the functions of musical awareness, spatial skills, emotional perception and holistic processing strategies. The LH is more involved with sequential ordering, coding and analytical processes. It has been shown that music is usually processed in the RH by naive listeners. It has also been shown that the development of cognitive skills is cyclical and moves around the brain, beginning in one hemisphere moving to the other and then culminating in bilateral parallel processing. It has also been stressed that these functions overlap and cognitive improvements depend on an coordinated integration of both hemispheres. Integration of hemispheric functions is the key point of this paper. The changes in cognitive development are not due to a shift in hemispheric specialization but rather changes in intra- and inter-hemispheric processing efficiency. This is demonstrated by the evidence that gifted students demonstrate and increase in electrical activity in both hemispheres when they tackle difficult problems in the area of their giftedness. Artistic training, provided throughout the developmental changes in hemispheric activity, physiologically strengthens the inter-hemispheric cortical connections that enables humans to make judgments, imagine possibilities, have volition, and apply specific functional skills in appropriate ways to novel situations. It is important to continually expose learners to artistic experience throughout development since maturation of cognitive skills depend on the cycling activity between the hemispheres. The RH hemisphere must be stimulated and exercised during the times that it is most open to developmental change in order for a fuller potential of the mind to be reached. It is important however to stress that full development is dependent upon encouraging the appreciation and expression of art for its own sake. Or rather, not only in service of other academic disciplines (math, writing, humanities). Hamblin (1995) argues against this tendency saying that these claims are often overblown and actually backfire. This paper proposes that artistic develop does help develop critical-thinking skills: the ability to pose questions, analyze evidence, consider hypothesis and defend points of view (Hamblin, 1995). These skills are obviously necessary for all areas of study and a life time. However, the direct causal relationships we have demonstrated between improving spatial skills by listening to Mozart should not be used to encourage teaching music instead of math. Cornock writes, "Although the balance between the verbal(mathematical) and tacit(integrative) components of consciousness appears to be influenced by educational experience, the challenge is to encourage coordinated development rather than to find ways of training specialized functional areas" (Cornock, pg. 153). The effort here has been to justify the importance of supporting artistic training throughout cognitive development. It should not be considered superfluous, provided only when funding allows. It is as necessary as writing and arithmetic to the development of the human brain. References Barkoczi, Ilona (1985) Psychological examination of the kodaly method of musical training. In Kardos, L, (Ed) Studies in Creativity, 109 - 162. Cornock, S., (1984) Implications of lateralization of brain function for art education: a critical review, Educational Psychology, 2, No. 2, 139- 153. Critchley, M., & Henson, R.A., (Eds) Music and The Brain: Studies in the Neurology of Music, London, William Heinemann Medical Books Limited. Damasio, A., & Damasio, H., (1977) Musical faculty and cerebral dominance.In Critchley, M., & Henson, R.A., (Eds) Music and The Brain: Studies in the Neurology of Music, London, William Heinemann Medical Books Limited, 141-155. Davidson, R.J., & Schwartz G.E., (1977) The influence of musical training on patterns of EEG asymmentry during musical and non-musical self- generation tasks, Psychophysiology, 14, No. 1, 58-63. Elliot, P.C., (1986), Right (or left) brain cognition, wrong metaphor for creative behavior: it is prefrontal lobe volition that makes the (human/humane) difference in the release of creative potential, The Journal of Creative Behavior, 20, No. 3, 202-214. Fischer, K. & Rose, S.P., (1994) Dynamic development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: a framework for theory and research. In Fischer, K. & Dawson, G. (Eds.) Human Behavior and the Developing Brain, New York, Guilford Press, 3 - 66. Gardiner M.F., Fox, A., Knowles F., & Jeffrey D., (1996) Learning improved by arts training, Nature , 381, May 23, pg. 284. Goldberg, E., & Costa, L.D. (1981) Hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems, Brain and Language, 14, 144-173. Goleman, Daniel, (1995) Emotional Intelligence, New York, Bantam Books. Hamblin, K. (1995) Theories and research that support art instruction for instrumental outcomes, Theories Into Practice, 32, No. 4, 191-198. Harth, E., (1993) The Creative Loop, New York, Addison-Wesley, pg 94. Hassler, M., (1990) Functional cerebral asymmentries and cognitive abilities in musicians, painters, and controls, Brain And Cognition ,13, 1-17. Henson, R.A., (1988) Neurological Aspects of Musical Experience. In Critchley, M., & Henson, R.A., (Eds) Music and The Brain: Studies in the Neurology of Music, London, William Heinemann Medical Books Limited, 3 - 21. Hudspeth, C., (1986) The cognitive and behavioral consequences of using music and poetry in a fourth grade language arts classroom, (Ph.D. dissertation.) Texas Womans' University, Denton, Texas,Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, (8-1), 2884. Joseph, R., (1988) The right cerebral hemisphere: emotion, music, visual-spatial skills, body-image, dreams, and awareness, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, No. 5, 630 -661. Kalliopuska, M., & Ruokonen, I., (1993) A study with a follow-up of hte effects of music education on holistic development of empathy, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 131-137. Licht, R., Bakker, D.J., Kok, A., & Bouma, A., (1988) The development of lateral event-related potentials (ERPs) related to word naming: a four year longitudinal study, Neuropsychologia, 26, No. 2, 327-340. Oboyle, M., Alexander, J., & Benbow, C., (1991) Enhanced right hemisphere activiation in the mathematically precocious: A preliminary EEG investigation, Brain and Cognition,17, No. 2, 138 -153 Ostwald, P., & Morrison, D., (1988) Music in the organization of childhood experience. In Morrison, D. (Ed), Organizing Early Experience: Imagination and Cognition in Childhood, Amityville, NY, Baywood Publishing Co., 54-73. Rauscher F., Shaw, G.L., Levine L.J., Ky, K.N., & Wright, E.L.(1994) Music and spatial task performance: a causal relationship. Presented at the American Psychological Assocaition 102nd Annual Convention in LA, CA. Rauscher, F.H., Shaw, G.l., & Ky, K.N., (1993) Music and spatial task performance, Nature, 365, October 14, pg. 611. Rideout, B.E., & Laubach, C., (1996) EEG correlates of enhanced spatial performance following exposure to music, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 427-432. Roberts, T., & Kraft, R.H., (1989) Developmental differences in the relationship between reading comprehension and hemispheric alpha patterns: an EEG study, Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, No. 3, 322-328. Samson, S., & Zatorre, R.J., (1992) Learning and retention of melodic and verbal information after unilateral temporal lobectomy, Neuropsychologia, 30, No. 9, 815-826. Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H., (1995) In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians, Science, 267, February 3, 699-701. Shreeve, J. (1996) Music of the hemispheres, Discover, October 1996, 90-100. Springer, S., & Deutsch, G. (1985), Left Brain, Right Brain: Revised Edition, New York, W.H. Freeman and Company, pages 187 - 204 (The development of asymmetry) and pages 235 - 251 (Hemisphericity, education and altered states). Thatcher, R.W., (1994) Cyclic cortical reorganization: origins of human cognitive development. In Fischer, K. & Dawson, G. (Eds.) Human Behavior and the Developing Brain, New York, Guilford Press, 232 - 267. Vincent, M.C., & Merrion, M. (1990) The musical mind considered: a new frontier, Design for Arts in Education, 92, No 1, Sept/Oct,11 - 18. Welch, N., (1995) Schools, Communities and the Arts: A Research Compendium, Arizona, Morrison Institute for Public Policy. Wertheim, N., (1977) Is there anatomical localisation for musical faculties?. In Critchley, M., & Henson, R.A., (Eds) Music and The Brain: Studies in the Neurology of Music, London, William Heinemann Medical Books Limited, 282 - 297. back to top of page